Dictionary Definition
ethology n : the branch of zoology that studies
the behavior of animals in their natural habitats
User Contributed Dictionary
English
Related terms
Translations
study of human and animal behaviour
- Czech: etologie
- Finnish: käyttäytymistiede; (study of animal behaviour) eläinpsykologia, etologia
- French: éthologie
- Ido: etologio
- Novial: etologia
Extensive Definition
Ethology (from Greek: ήθος, ethos, "character";
and λόγος, logos, "knowledge") is the scientific study of animal behavior, and a branch of
zoology (not to be
confused with ethnology).
Although many naturalists have studied aspects of
animal behavior through the centuries, the modern discipline of
ethology is usually considered to have arisen with the work in the
1960s of Dutch biologist Nikolaas
Tinbergen and Austrian biologist Konrad
Lorenz, joint winners of the 1973 Nobel Prize
in Biology. Ethology is a combination of laboratory and field
science, with strong ties to certain other disciplines — e.g.,
neuroanatomy,
ecology, evolution. Ethologists are
typically interested in a behavioral process rather than in a
particular animal group and often study one type of behavior (e.g.,
aggression) in a number of unrelated animals.
The desire to understand the animal world has
made ethology a rapidly growing field, and since the turn of the
21st century, many prior understandings related to diverse fields
such as animal
communication, personal symbolic name use, animal
emotions, animal culture and learning,
and even sexual
conduct, long thought to be well understood, have been
revolutionized, as have new fields such as neuroethology.
Etymology
The term "ethology" is derived from the Greek word
"èthos" (ήθος), meaning "character". Other words derived from the
Greek word "ethos" include "ethics" and "ethical". The term was
first popularized in English by the American myrmecologist William
Morton Wheeler in 1902. (An earlier, slightly different sense
of the term was proposed by John
Stuart Mill in his 1843 System of Logic. He recommended the
development of a new science, "ethology," whose purpose would be
the explanation of individual and national differences in
character, on the basis of associationistic psychology. This use of the
word was never adopted.)
Differences and similarities with comparative psychology
Comparative
psychology also studies animal behaviour, but, as opposed to
ethology, construes its study as a branch of psychology rather than as one
of biology.
Historically, where comparative psychology sees the study of animal
behaviour in the context of what is known about human psychology,
ethology sees the study of animal behaviour in the context of what
is known about animal anatomy, physiology, neurobiology, and phylogenetic history. This
distinction is not representative of the current state of the
field. Furthermore, early comparative psychologists concentrated on
the study of learning and tended to look at behaviour in artificial
situations, whereas early ethologists concentrated on behaviour in
natural situations, tending to describe it as instinctive. The two
approaches are complementary rather than competitive, but they do
lead to different perspectives and sometimes to conflicts of
opinion about matters of substance. In addition, for most of the
twentieth century, comparative psychology developed most strongly
in North
America, while ethology was stronger in Europe, and this led
to different emphases as well as somewhat differing philosophical
underpinnings in the two disciplines. A practical difference is
that early comparative psychologists concentrated on gaining
extensive knowledge of the behaviour of very few species, while ethologists were
more interested in gaining knowledge of behaviour in a wide range
of species in order to be able to make principled comparisons
across taxonomic
groups. Ethologists have made much more use of a truly comparative
method than comparative psychologists ever have. Despite the
historical divergence, most ethologists (as opposed to behavioural
ecologists), at least in North America, teach in psychology
departments. It is a strong belief among scientists that the
mechanisms on which behavioural processes are based are the same
that rule the evolution of the living species: there is therefore a
strong connection between these two fields.
Scala Naturae and Lamarck's theories
Until the 18th century, the most common theory
among scientists was still the Scala
Naturae proposed by Aristotle:
according to this theory, the living beings were classified on an
ideal pyramid in which
the simplest animals were occupying the lower floors, and then
complexity would raise progressively until the top, which was
occupied by the human beings. There was also an avant-garde
group of biologists who were refusing the Aristotelian theory for a
more anthropocentric one, according to which all living beings were
created by God to serve mankind, and would behave accordingly. A
well-radicated opinion in the common sense
of the time in the Western world was that animal species were
eternal and immutable, created with a specific purpose, as this
seemed the only possible explanation for the incredible variety of
the living beings and their surprising adaptation to their habitat.
The first biologist elaborating a complex evolution theory was
Jean-Baptiste
Lamarck (1744-1829). His theory was substantially made of two
statements: the first is that animal organs and behaviour can
change according to the way they are being used, and that those
characteristics are capable of being transmitted from one
generation to the next (well-known is the example of the giraffe whose neck becomes
longer while trying to reach the upper leaves of a tree). The
second affirmation is that each and every living organism, human
beings included, tends to reach a greater level of perfection. At
the time of his journey for the Galapagos
Islands, Charles
Darwin was well aware of Lamarck's theories and was deeply
influenced by them.
Theory of evolution by natural selection and the beginnings of ethology
Because ethology is understood as a branch of
biology, ethologists have been particularly concerned with the
evolution of behaviour
and the understanding of behaviour in terms of the theory of
natural
selection. In one sense, the first modern ethologist was
Charles
Darwin, whose book, The Expression of the Emotions in Man and
Animals, has influenced many ethologists. He pursued his interest
in behaviour by encouraging his protégé George
Romanes, who investigated animal learning and intelligence
using an anthropomorphic method, anecdotal
cognitivism, that did not gain scientific support.
Other early ethologists, such as Oskar
Heinroth and Julian
Huxley, instead concentrated on behaviours that can be called
instinctive, or natural, in that they occur in all members of a
species under specified circumstances. Their first step in studying
the behaviour of a new species was to construct an ethogram (a
description of the main types of natural behaviour with their
frequencies of occurrence). This approach provided an objective,
cumulative base of data about behaviour, which subsequent
researchers could check and build on.
Fixed action patterns and animal communication
An important step, associated with the name of
Konrad
Lorenz though probably due more to his teacher, Oskar
Heinroth, was the identification of fixed
action patterns (FAPs). Lorenz popularized FAPs as instinctive
responses that would occur reliably in the presence of identifiable
stimuli (called sign stimuli or releasing stimuli). These FAPs
could then be compared across species, and the similarities and
differences between behaviour could be easily compared with the
similarities and differences in morphology.
An important and much quoted study of the Anatidae (ducks
and geese) by Heinroth used this technique. The ethologists noted
that the stimuli that released FAPs were commonly features of the
appearance or behaviour of other members of their own species, and
they were able to show how important forms of animal
communication could be mediated by a few simple FAPs. The most
sophisticated investigation of this kind was the study by Karl von
Frisch of the so-called "dance language" underlying
bee communication. Lorenz developed an interesting theory of
the evolution of animal communication based on his observations of
the nature of fixed action patterns and the circumstances in which
animals emit them.
Instinct
Modern psychoanalysis defines
instinct as an impulse
which forces an individual to accomplish a task through pre-defined
mental schemes, behaviours that are not caused by the usual
learning process nor personal choice. In ethology, by instinct we mean a series of
rigid and predictable actions and behavioural schemes which go
under the term of fixed
action patterns. Such schemes are only acted when a precise
stimulating signal is present. When such signals act as
communication among members of the same species, they go under the
name of releasers.
Notable examples of releasers are, in many bird species, the beak
movements by the newborns, which stimulates the mother's
regurgitating process to feed the child. Another well known case is
the classic experiments by Tinbergen and Lorenz on
the Graylag
Goose. Like similar waterfowl, it will roll a
displaced egg near
its nest back to the others with its beak. The sight of the
displaced egg triggers this mechanism. If the egg is taken away,
the animal continues with the behavior, pulling its head back as if
an imaginary egg is still being maneuvered by the underside of its
beak. However, it will also attempt to move other egg shaped
objects, such as a golf ball, door knob, or even an egg too large
to have possibly been laid by the goose itself (a supernormal
stimulus). As made obvious by this last example, however, a
behaviour only made of fixed
action patterns would result particularly rigid and
inefficient, reducing the probabilities of survival and reproduction. The learning
process has therefore a great importance, as the ability to change
the individual's responses change based on its experience. It can
be said that the more the brain is complex and the life of
the individual long, the more its behaviour will result
"intelligent" (in the sense of guided by experience rather than
rigid FAPs).
Learning
The learning process may take place in many ways,
one of the most elementary is assuefaction. This process consists
in ignoring a persistent or useless stimuli. An example of learning
by assuefaction is the one observed in squirrels: when one of them
feels in danger, the others hear its signal and go to the nearest
repair. However, if the signal comes from an individual who has
performed a big number of false
alarms, his signal will be ignored.
Another common way of learning is by association,
where a stimuli is, based on the experience, linked to another one
which may not have anything to do with the first one. The first
studies of associative learning were made by Russian physiologist Ivan Pavlov.
An example of associative behaviour is observed when a common
goldfish goes close to the water surface whenever a human is going
to feed it, or the excitement of a dog whenever it sees a collar as a prelude for a walk.
The associative learning process is linked to the necessity of
developing discriminatory capacities, that is, the faculty of
making meaningful choices. Being able to discriminate the members
of your own species is of fundamental importance for the
reproductive success. Such discrimination can be based on a number
of factors: in many species (among which birds), however, this important
type of learning only takes place in a very limited period of time.
This kind of learning is called imprinting.
Imprinting
A second important finding of Lorenz concerned the early learning of young nidifugous birds, a process he called imprinting. Lorenz observed that the young of birds such as geese and chickens spontaneously followed their mothers from almost the first day after they were hatched, and he discovered that this response could be imitated by an arbitrary stimulus if the eggs were incubated artificially and the stimulus was presented during a critical period (a less temporally constrained period is called a sensitive period) that continued for a few days after hatching.Imitation
Finally, imitation is often a big part
of the learning process. A well-documented example of imitative
learning is that of macaques in Hachijojima
island, Japan. These primates used to live in the
inland forest until the 60s, when a group of researchers started
giving them some potatoes on the beach: soon they started venturing
onto the beach, picking the potatoes from the sand, and cleaning
and eating them. About one year later, an individual was observed
bringing a potato to the sea, putting it into the water with one
hand, and cleaning it with the other. Her behaviour was soon
imitated by the individuals living in contact with her; when they
gave birth, they taught this practice to their children.
Mating and the fight for supremacy
The individual reproduction is with no
doubt the most important phase in the proliferation of the
species: for this reason, we can often observe complex mating ritual, which can reach a high
level of complexity even if they are often regarded as fixed
action patterns (FAPs). The Stickleback's
complex mating ritual was studied by Niko
Tinbergen and is regarded as a notable example of a FAP. Often
in social
life, males are fighting for the right of reproducing
themselves as well as social
supremacy. Such behaviours are common among mammals.
A common example of fight for social and sexual
supremacy is the so-called pecking
order among poultry.
A pecking order is established every time a group of poultry
co-lives for a certain amount of time. In each of these groups, a
chicken is dominating among the others and can peck before anyone
else without being pecked. A second chicken can peck all the others
but the first, and so on. The chicken in the higher levels can be
easily distinguished for their well-cured aspect, as opposed to the
ones in the lower levels. During the period in which the pecking
order is establishing, frequent and violent fights can happen, but
once it is established it is only broken when other individuals are
entering the group, in which case the pecking order has to be
established from scratch.
Living in groups
Several animal species, including humans, tend to
live in groups. Group
size is a major aspect of their social environment. Social life
is probably a complex and effective survival strategy. It may be
regarded as a sort of symbiosis among individuals of
the same species: a society is composed of a group
of individuals belonging to the same species living in a
well-defined rule on food
management, role assignments and reciprocal dependence.
The situation is, actually much more complex than
it looks. When biologists interested in
evolution
theory first started examining the social behaviour, some
apparently unanswerable questions came up. How could, for instance,
the birth of sterile
casts, like in bees, be explained through an
evolving mechanism which emphasizes the reproductive success of as
many individuals as possible? Why, among animals living in small
groups like squirrels,
would an individual risk its own life to save the rest of the
group? These behaviours are examples of altruism. Of course, not all
behaviours are altruistic, as shown in the table below. Notably,
revengeful behaviour is claimed to have been observed exclusively
in Homo
sapiens.
The existence of egoism through natural selection
doesn't pose any question to the evolution
theory and is, on the contrary, fully justified by it, as well
as for the cooperative behaviour. It is much harder to understand
the mechanism through which the altruistic behaviour
initially developed.
Tinbergen's four questions for ethologists
Lorenz's collaborator, Niko Tinbergen, argued that ethology always needed to pay attention to four kinds of explanation in any instance of behaviour:- Function — How does the behaviour impact on the animal's chances of survival and reproduction?
- Causation — What are the stimuli that elicit the response, and how has it been modified by recent learning?
- Development — How does the behaviour change with age, and what early experiences are necessary for the behaviour to be shown?
- Evolutionary history — How does the behaviour compare with similar behaviour in related species, and how might it have arisen through the process of phylogeny?
Flowering
Through the work of Lorenz and Tinbergen,
ethology developed strongly in continental Europe in the years
before World War
II. After the war, Tinbergen moved to the University
of Oxford, and ethology became stronger in the UK, with
the additional influence of William
Thorpe, Robert
Hinde, and Patrick
Bateson at the Sub-department of Animal Behaviour of the
University
of Cambridge, located in the village of Madingley. In
this period, too, ethology began to develop strongly in North
America.
Lorenz, Tinbergen, and von Frisch were jointly
awarded the Nobel Prize
in 1973 for their work in developing ethology.
Ethology is now a well recognised scientific
discipline, and has a number of journals covering developments in
the subject, such as the Ethology
journal.
Social ethology and recent developments
In 1970, the English ethologist
John H. Crook published an important paper in which he
distinguished comparative ethology from social ethology, and argued
that much of the ethology that had existed so far was really
comparative ethology--looking at animals as individuals--whereas in
the future ethologists would need to concentrate on the behaviour
of social groups of animals and the social structure within
them.
Also in 1970, a book was published called "The
Social Contract: A Personal Inquiry into the Evolutionary Sources
of Order and Disorder" written by Robert
Ardrey. The book and study investigated animal behavior and
then compared human behavior as a similar phenomenon.
Indeed, E. O.
Wilson's book
Sociobiology: The New Synthesis appeared in 1975, and since
that time the study of behaviour has been much more concerned with
social aspects. It has also been driven by the stronger, but more
sophisticated, Darwinism associated with Wilson and Richard
Dawkins. The related development of behavioural
ecology has also helped transform ethology. Furthermore, a
substantial rapprochement with comparative psychology has occurred,
so the modern scientific study of behaviour offers a more or less
seamless spectrum of approaches – from animal
cognition to more traditional comparative
psychology, ethology, sociobiology and behavioural
ecology. Sociobiology has more recently developed into evolutionary
psychology.
Notes
- There are often mismatches between human senses and those of the organisms they are observing. To compensate, ethologists often reach all the way back to epistemology to give them the tools to predict and avoid misinterpretation of data.
- "Super-real object" is an object that causes an abnormally strong response in an animal. An example of this is the design of dummies that mimic and over-stress the key characteristics of individuals in certain species causing animals to direct behaviour to the super-real object and ignore the real object. A super-real object may cause pathologies and we can see many examples in humans (super-sweet food, super-big female traits, super-relaxing drugs, etc.). See the book, Foundations of Ethology by Konrad Lorenz.
- Deleuze draws upon the notions of ethology in his book "Spinoza: Practical Philosophy" to develop his ontology, most specifically in reference to the plane of immanence.
List of ethologists
People who have made notable contributions to the
field of ethology (many are comparative psychologists):
See also
References
External links
General
- Konrad Lorenz Institute for Evolution and Cognitive Research (KLI)
- Center for the Integrative Study of Animal behaviour (CISAB)
- Introduction to ethology
- Applied Ethology
- The International Society for Human Ethology — aims at promoting ethological perspectives in the scientific study of humans worldwide
- Abstracts of the XXIX Ethological Conference
Diagrams on Tinbergen's four questions
ethology in Arabic: إيثولوجيا
ethology in Bulgarian: Етология
ethology in Catalan: Etologia
ethology in Czech: Etologie
ethology in Danish: Etologi
ethology in German: Ethologie
ethology in Estonian: Etoloogia
ethology in Spanish: Etología
ethology in Esperanto: Etologio
ethology in French: Éthologie
ethology in Western Frisian:
Gedrachsbiology
ethology in Galician: Etoloxía
ethology in Korean: 행동 생물학
ethology in Indonesian: Etologi
ethology in Interlingua (International Auxiliary
Language Association): Ethologia
ethology in Italian: Etologia
ethology in Hebrew: אתולוגיה
ethology in Latvian: Etoloģija
ethology in Lithuanian: Etologija
ethology in Hungarian: Etológia
ethology in Dutch: Ethologie
ethology in Japanese: 動物行動学
ethology in Norwegian: Atferdsbiologi
ethology in Norwegian Nynorsk:
Åtferdsbiologi
ethology in Occitan (post 1500): Etologia
ethology in Polish: Etologia
ethology in Portuguese: Etologia
ethology in Romanian: Etologie
ethology in Russian: Этология
ethology in Simple English: Ethology
ethology in Slovak: Etológia
ethology in Slovenian: Etologija
ethology in Finnish: Etologia
ethology in Swedish: Etologi
ethology in Tagalog: Etolohiya
ethology in Tetum: Etolojia
ethology in Vietnamese: Tập tính học
ethology in Turkish: Etoloji
ethology in Ukrainian: Етологія
ethology in Urdu: حیوانی سلوکیات
ethology in Chinese: 动物行为学